Search or filter publications

Filter by type:

Filter by publication type

Filter by year:

to

Results

  • Showing results for:
  • Reset all filters

Search results

  • Conference paper
    Mason S, Zhang J, Rowe J, Barker R, Hampshire Aet al., 2014,

    FRONTOSTRIATAL ABNORMALITIES IN HUNTINGTON'S DISEASE: AN FMRI STUDY

    , 8th European-Huntington's-Disease-Network Plenary Meeting, Publisher: BMJ PUBLISHING GROUP, Pages: A45-A45, ISSN: 0022-3050
  • Journal article
    Jilka SR, Scott G, Ham T, Pickering A, Bonnelle V, Braga RM, Leech R, Sharp DJet al., 2014,

    Damage to the Salience Network and Interactions with the Default Mode Network

    , JOURNAL OF NEUROSCIENCE, Vol: 34, Pages: 10798-10807, ISSN: 0270-6474
  • Journal article
    Leech R, Scott G, Carhart-Harris R, Turkheimer F, Taylor-Robinson SD, Sharp DJet al., 2014,

    Spatial Dependencies between Large-Scale Brain Networks

    , PLoS ONE, Vol: 9

    <p>Functional neuroimaging reveals both increases (task-positive) and decreases (task-negative) in neural activation with many tasks. Many studies show a <italic>temporal</italic> relationship between task positive and task negative networks that is important for efficient cognitive functioning. Here we provide evidence for a <italic>spatial</italic> relationship between task positive and negative networks. There are strong spatial similarities between many reported task negative brain networks, termed the default mode network, which is typically assumed to be a spatially fixed network. However, this is not the case. The spatial structure of the DMN varies depending on what specific task is being performed. We test whether there is a fundamental <italic>spatial</italic> relationship between task positive and negative networks. Specifically, we hypothesize that the distance between task positive and negative voxels is consistent despite different spatial patterns of activation and deactivation evoked by different cognitive tasks. We show significantly reduced variability in the distance between within-condition task positive and task negative voxels than across-condition distances for four different sensory, motor and cognitive tasks - implying that deactivation patterns are spatially dependent on activation patterns (and <italic>vice versa</italic>), and that both are modulated by specific task demands. We also show a similar relationship between positively and negatively correlated networks from a third ‘rest’ dataset, in the absence of a specific task. We propose that this spatial relationship may be the macroscopic analogue of microscopic neuronal organization reported in sensory cortical systems, and that this organization may reflect homeostatic plasticity necessary for efficient brain function.</p>

  • Journal article
    Scholtz S, Miras AD, Chhina N, Prechtl CG, Sleeth ML, Daud NM, Ismail NA, Durighel G, Ahmed AR, Olbers T, Vincent RP, Alaghband-Zadeh J, Ghatei MA, Waldman AD, Frost GS, Bell JD, le Roux CW, Goldstone APet al., 2014,

    Obese patients after gastric bypass surgery have lower brain-hedonic responses to food than after gastric banding

    , GUT, Vol: 63, Pages: 891-902, ISSN: 0017-5749
  • Journal article
    Vijayan R, Scott G, Brownlie W, 2014,

    Out of sight, but not out of mind? Greater reported pain in patients who spontaneously look away during venepuncture

    , EJP, Pages: n/a-n/a, ISSN: 1532-2149
  • Journal article
    Goldstone AP, Prechtl CG, Scholtz S, Miras AD, Chhina N, Durighel G, Deliran SS, Beckmann C, Ghatei MA, Ashby DR, Waldman AD, Gaylinn BD, Thorner MO, Frost GS, Bloom SR, Bell JDet al., 2014,

    Ghrelin mimics fasting to enhance human hedonic, orbitofrontal cortex, and hippocampal responses to food

    , AMERICAN JOURNAL OF CLINICAL NUTRITION, Vol: 99, Pages: 1319-1330, ISSN: 0002-9165
  • Journal article
    Daud NM, Ismail NA, Thomas EL, Fitzpatrick JA, Bell JD, Swann JR, Costabile A, Childs CE, Pedersen C, Goldstone AP, Frost GSet al., 2014,

    The Impact of Oligofructose on Stimulation of Gut Hormones, Appetite Regulation and Adiposity

    , OBESITY, Vol: 22, Pages: 1430-1438, ISSN: 1930-7381
  • Journal article
    Erika-Florence M, Leech R, Hampshire A, 2014,

    A functional network perspective on response inhibition and attentional control

    , NATURE COMMUNICATIONS, Vol: 5, ISSN: 2041-1723
  • Journal article
    Miras AD, Scholtz S, Chhina N, Durighel G, Bell JD, Le Roux C, Goldstone APet al., 2014,

    Role for Increased Plasma PYY and GLP-1 in Reducing Anticipatory Food Reward after Gastric Bypass Surgery

    , ENDOCRINE REVIEWS, Vol: 35, ISSN: 0163-769X
  • Journal article
    Herrera PM, Speranza M, Hampshire A, Bekinschtein TAet al., 2014,

    Monetary rewards modulate inhibitory control

    , Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, Vol: 8

    The ability to override a dominant response, often referred to as behavioral inhibition, is considered a key element of executive cognition. Poor behavioral inhibition is a defining characteristic of several neurological and psychiatric populations. Recently, there has been increasing interest in the motivational dimension of behavioral inhibition, with some experiments incorporating emotional contingencies in classical inhibitory paradigms such as the Go/NoGo and Stop Signal Tasks (SSTs). Several studies have reported a positive modulatory effect of reward on performance in pathological conditions such as substance abuse, pathological gambling, and Attention Deficit Hyperactive Disorder (ADHD). However, experiments that directly investigate the modulatory effects of reward magnitudes on the performance of inhibitory tasks are scarce and little is known about the finer grained relationship between motivation and inhibitory control. Here we probed the effect of reward magnitude and context on behavioral inhibition with three modified versions of the widely used SST. The pilot study compared inhibition performance during six blocks alternating neutral feedback, low, medium, and high monetary rewards. Study One compared increasing vs. decreasing rewards, with low, high rewards, and neutral feedback; whilst Study Two compared low and high reward magnitudes alone also in an increasing and decreasing reward design. The reward magnitude effect was not demonstrated in the pilot study, probably due to a learning effect induced by practice in this lengthy task. The reward effect per se was weak but the context (order of reward) was clearly suggested in Study One, and was particularly strongly confirmed in study two. In addition, these findings revealed a "kick start effect" over global performance measures. Specifically, there was a long lasting improvement in performance throughout the task when participants received the highest reward magnitudes at the beginning o

This data is extracted from the Web of Science and reproduced under a licence from Thomson Reuters. You may not copy or re-distribute this data in whole or in part without the written consent of the Science business of Thomson Reuters.

Request URL: http://www.imperial.ac.uk:80/respub/WEB-INF/jsp/search-t4-html.jsp Request URI: /respub/WEB-INF/jsp/search-t4-html.jsp Query String: id=1053&limit=10&page=23&respub-action=search.html Current Millis: 1723058291727 Current Time: Wed Aug 07 20:18:11 BST 2024